Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2022 Apr 27:16:838228.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.838228. eCollection 2022.

Contributions of the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Val158Met Polymorphism to Changes in Brain Iron Across Adulthood and Their Relationships to Working Memory

Affiliations

Contributions of the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Val158Met Polymorphism to Changes in Brain Iron Across Adulthood and Their Relationships to Working Memory

Jonatan Gustavsson et al. Front Hum Neurosci. .

Abstract

Ageing is associated with excessive free brain iron, which may induce oxidative stress and neuroinflammation, likely causing cognitive deficits. Lack of dopamine may be a factor behind the increase of iron with advancing age, as it has an important role in cellular iron homoeostasis. We investigated the effect of COMT Val 158 Met (rs4680), a polymorphism crucial for dopamine degradation and proxy for endogenous dopamine, on iron accumulation and working memory in a longitudinal lifespan sample (n = 208, age 20-79 at baseline, mean follow-up time = 2.75 years) using structural equation modelling. Approximation of iron content was assessed using quantitative susceptibility mapping in striatum and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Iron accumulated in both striatum and DLPFC during the follow-up period. Greater iron accumulation in DLPFC was associated with more deleterious change in working memory. Older (age 50-79) Val homozygotes (with presumably lower endogenous dopamine) accumulated more iron than older Met carriers in both striatum and DLPFC, no such differences were observed among younger adults (age 20-49). In conclusion, individual differences in genetic predisposition related to low dopamine levels increase iron accumulation, which in turn may trigger deleterious change in working memory. Future studies are needed to better understand how dopamine may modulate iron accumulation across the human lifespan.

Keywords: COMT; QSM; SEM; ageing; brain iron; dopamine; longitudinal; working memory.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
(A) Example of a QSM (quantitative susceptibility mapping) image. Higher signal intensity denotes higher iron load. Striatum is represented by caudate (red outline) and putamen (blue outline). (B) Image illustrating dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with rostral middle-frontal cortex in blue colour and caudal middle-frontal cortex in red colour.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Graphical representation of the change regression model for iron accumulation and working memory change in (A) striatum and (B) DLPFC. Observed variables are indicated by boxes and latent factors are indicated by circles. Regressions are indicated with one-headed arrows and covariances with two-headed arrows. Figures present standardised parameter estimates. All models were adjusted for age, sex, and education, as well as change in grey-matter volume in case of iron accumulation. T1, baseline; T2, follow-up; e, error term; WM, working memory; BD, binding task; 2B, 2-back task; 3B, 3-back task. (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001).
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Accumulation of iron [susceptibility increase in parts per million (ppm)] in (A,B) striatum and (C,D) DLPFC. Error bars ± 1 SE. * indicates significant difference in iron accumulation based on structural equation modeling (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001).
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Association between changes in working memory and iron accumulation [susceptibility increase in parts per million (ppm)]. Top row: Younger (A) Met/Met, (B) Met/Val, and (C) Val/Val carriers. Bottom row: Older (D) Met/Met, (E) Met/Val, and (F) Val/Val carriers. Plots are based on imputed factor scores. * indicates marginal multivariate outlier whose exclusion led to a marginally significant association (see text).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Acosta-Cabronero J., Betts M. J., Cardenas-Blanco A., Yang S., Nestor P. J. (2016). In vivo MRI mapping of brain iron deposition across the adult lifespan. J. Neurosci. 36 364–374. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1907-15.2016 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Adamo D. E., Daugherty A. M., Raz N. (2014). Grasp force matching and brain iron content estimated in vivo in older women. Brain Imaging Behav. 8 579–587. 10.1007/s11682-013-9284-6 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Andersson J., Jenkinson M., Smith S. (2007). Non-Linear Registration aka Spatial Normalisation. FMRIB Technial Report TR07JA2. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Bäckman L., Lindenberger U., Li S. C., Nyberg L. (2010). Linking cognitive aging to alterations in dopamine neurotransmitter functioning: recent data and future avenues. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34 670–677. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.008 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bäckman L., Nyberg L., Lindenberger U., Li S. C., Farde L. (2006). The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: current status and future prospects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30 791–807. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.005 - DOI - PubMed