Skip to main content

Steps in the design, development and formative evaluation of obesity prevention-related behavior change trials

Abstract

Obesity prevention interventions through dietary and physical activity change have generally not been effective. Limitations on possible program effectiveness are herein identified at every step in the mediating variable model, a generic conceptual framework for understanding how interventions may promote behavior change. To minimize these problems, and thereby enhance likely intervention effectiveness, four sequential types of formative studies are proposed: targeted behavior validation, targeted mediator validation, intervention procedure validation, and pilot feasibility intervention. Implementing these studies would establish the relationships at each step in the mediating variable model, thereby maximizing the likelihood that an intervention would work and its effects would be detected. Building consensus among researchers, funding agencies, and journal editors on distinct intervention development studies should avoid identified limitations and move the field forward.

Background

Obesity is at epidemic proportions in the United States (US)[1] and growing around the world[2]. While there has been a call for increased emphasis on lifestyle factors for obesity prevention[3], there is a crisis in the conduct of community interventions for promoting dietary and physical activity change. Repeated reviews have indicated that most obesity prevention interventions have attained only limited or no behavioral changes; they have rarely impacted the targeted physiological or anthropometric health outcomes; and no common patterns of effect have emerged to differentiate the few successful from unsuccessful programs [4–6]. This situation is not of recent vintage[7]. Also, there is no thoroughly supported evidence-based guidance on what should be done[8]. Many government agencies are experiencing pressures to "act" (i.e. implement community obesity prevention programs) and important efforts have been made to map reasonable courses of community wide action[9]. However, premature action with repeated failures is a waste of public resources and may lead to loss of public confidence in community interventions.

The response of the obesity prevention research community needs to be more carefully planned to build a cumulative science of behavior change and evidence-based guidance[10]. The complexity of influences on adiposity and variations in influences by socioeconomic, and geographic factors have been outlined[9]. Researchers need to systematically develop dietary and physical activity behavior change programs that take this complexity into account by using the best available behavioral, social, and ecological theories and methods[11]. Concurrent with such a shift, funding agencies, their peer reviewers and journal editors should accept clearly defined research study steps in the design, development, and formative evaluation of programs that they are willing to fund and publish. Although several groups have addressed the need for a formative phase in developing interventions[11, 12], this paper proposes a series of four formative studies that should be conducted to avoid the method and conceptual problems of earlier efforts, and thereby build a stronger foundation to design effective interventions and more likely detect their effects.

Two large well funded studies were selected to provide examples of top obesity prevention intervention efforts: an elementary school-based study (a very popular intervention channel for reaching children) in a high risk group[13] and a large national media based study (commonly believed to be a channel for large public health benefit)[14]. The school-based project did not achieve intervention related differences in some indicator of body composition, while the media project detected differences across exposure groups.

Mediating Variable Model

The ecological, social, and psychological sciences offer an understanding of why people engage in the behaviors they do. The mediating variable model of behavior change (see Figure 1) posits that intervention programs attain behavior change by inducing changes in mediating variables (that come from the ecological, social, and psychological theories), and changes in these mediating variables induce relatively stable changes in behavior[15] in an approximately linear fashion. Implications of the mediating variable model are that (a) behaviors need to be selected that are maximally and causally related to the health outcomes of concern (or else the health problems will not change); (b) ecological, social and psychological mediators (in the context of known biology) need to be selected that are maximally and causally related to the behavior (otherwise change in mediators will not result in sufficiently large changes in behavior); (c) mediators need to be selected that are highly predictive of the behavior (otherwise substantial changes in the mediators may result in only small or no changes in the behavior); and (d) intervention procedures need to be identified or developed that effectively manipulate the mediators at acceptable levels (or else participants will not receive an effective intervention dose). Problems in previous intervention programs and their evaluations have been identified at each stage and component of the mediating variable model, including a) targeted behaviors were not related to health outcomes in target groups[16]; b) inadequate measurement of the behavior impeded detecting a relationship with adiposity[17, 18]; c) hypothesized mediating variables were unrelated to, or even suppressed changes in, the behavior[19]; d) poor quality of the measure of the mediating variable inhibited detecting relationships [20]; e) interventions did not impact mediator(s)[15, 21]; and f) inadequate intervention implementation led to failure to detect intervention effects on the mediators[22]. A stepwise approach to designing and developing dietary and physical activity behavior change interventions should minimize these limitations and, thereby, maximize the likelihood of success.

Figure 1
figure 1

Model of Mediation and Moderation of Intervention Outcomes

General Issues in the Design of Behavior Change Intervention

Behavior change interventions must be developed to meet the needs and capitalize on the strengths of specific groups of people (i.e. specific demographic characteristics), using a specific channel (i.e. a delivery method). Thus, a pre-step in designing a behavior change intervention is to select a targeted population (e.g., all 9–11 year old children) using a specific channel (e.g., elementary schools). The choice of channel engages both its strengths and limitations[23, 24]. For example, using tribal schools enables reaching large numbers of Native American students, but also encounter layers of approval (and delay) from tribal councils[13]; use of the larger media can reach large numbers of citizens nationwide, but could involve inconsistent messaging when developed by media writers, imposes difficulties in measuring heights and weights from participants, and makes control groups and randomization practically impossible[14].

Adiposity indicators (e.g., BMI, waist circumference, skinfolds, waist to height ratio[25]) must be selected carefully because a) different program outcomes could be obtained with different indicators[26]; b) in some populations the selected indicator and adiposity do not seem to be interrelated as expected[27, 28]; and c) the indicators differentially relate to key socio-demographic factors[29]. Raw BMI (as opposed to gender and age specific centiles or z scores) may be the best metric when assessing changes[30]. One of the example programs generated a population specific multiple indicator regression equation to predict percent body fat[31] to minimize the limitations of using BMI alone[27]. This should provide a model for others to consider. The other[14], however, used self reported height and weight which has severe problems with accuracy. Inadequate measures, even with large samples, make it difficult to detect effects, and may even lead to erroneous conclusions[32].

Formative Step A. Targeted Behavior Validation

Behaviors should be targeted for change that are causally and substantially related to the health problem. While some behaviors are clearly related to a health problem (e.g., cigarette smoking and lung cancer), obesity does not have clearly empirically verified universal behavioral causes[16]. For example, the literature has been reviewed on the relationship of sweetened beverage consumption to obesity[16]. Limitations in the research methods to date would not permit definite conclusions about the relationship[16]. At one time dietary fat intake was considered the primary cause of obesity [33], but that perception has changed as well[34]. The behavior to health outcome relationship may exist in certain groups (e.g., elementary school aged children), but not the one targeted for intervention (e.g., pre-school children[35]). Single nutrients or food groups thought to be predictive of obesity (e.g. sucrose) may simply be indicative of an overall poor diet quality[36]. People tend to eat multiple foods organized into consistent patterns overtime[37]. There may be value in targeting patterns of dietary intake, rather than specific nutrients or foods, since these may be more strongly related to the health outcomes[38]. However, even here, the relationships in certain groups don't always emerge as expected (e.g., the prudent diet predisposed to breast cancer among women in New Mexico[39], and the vegetable rich pattern was associated with obesity among the Chinese[40]). The place where people eat (e.g., restaurants, friends' homes) may be a marker of poorer dietary practices and provide a useful intervention target[41].

Similar problems exist in regard to selection of a targeted level of physical activity. Although 60 min of moderate to vigorous physical activity per day 3 to 5 days per week has been prescribed as the desirable level to prevent obesity and other chronic illnesses[42], children engaging in that level (or more) over a three year period (using an objective measure) were not leaner than children who did not[43]. There have been disagreements about whether one should measure physical activity or fitness[44], and concerns about the quality of measurement especially when using self-reports[45]. One of our example programs detected change in self-reported, but not objectively assessed, physical activity, suggesting some self-report bias[13].

There have been a number of efforts to identify the factors accounting for the obesity epidemic[16, 46], with no clear strong consistent findings. Poor diet and physical inactivity may not be primary causes of the current obesity epidemic[47]. Lack of self control of satiety (not specific foods)[48] may be a major contributor. Alternatively, obesity appears likely influenced by a multitude of neither sufficient nor necessary factors that interact and sometimes trigger compensatory behaviors. For example, lower energy intake from specific foods may be compensated by an equal increase of energy intake from other nutrients and foods[49], or increases in energy expenditure[50]. The fact that very large samples (hundreds of thousands) are needed to detect biologically plausible dietary and physical activity behavior to obesity relationships[51] likely indicates a) compensatory behaviors, b) heterogeneity of effects across population subgroups, and c) poor quality of measures of behavior and health outcomes. One example program using more objective observational measures of intake at school lunch detected differences in fat intake, but not total energy, suggesting compensation for energy dense fat with other foods[13].

Research clearly delineating the causes of obesity may be the most important contribution to obesity prevention behavior change research[52] at this time. Formative Step A research should assess the metric qualities (reliability, validity) of the methods employed in their hands; identify possible confounders and moderators of cross-sectional or, optimally, longitudinal behavior-outcome relationships (e.g., physical activity moderating a caloric intake to adiposity relationship); and include measures of common response biases (e.g social desirability of response).

A realistic assessment must be made of the extent of change in caloric imbalance necessary to redress the obesity problem in the target population, and thereby the number and extent of behavior changes required[53]. To minimize the likelihood of selecting ineffective behaviors to change in intervention, the possible behavioral influences on obesity should be identified, and one or more selected for change that are most strongly related to obesity; while paying attention to possible compensatory effects, with some evidence that a) the behaviors are amendable to change; b) there are known models for why people do those behaviors (sources of mediating variables); and c) changes in the behaviors can be reasonably easily and precisely measured to evaluate the outcome.

Investigators need to address how the intervention channel (e.g., the elementary school) influences the behavior (e.g., foods offered in school breakfast, school lunch, alternative food line, vending machines), the environment of the channels (e.g., proximity of the schools to fast food stores), and especially the behaviors performed/exhibited in those environments. Otherwise promising interventions may fail because they ignored channel and channel-environment effects[23].

Often the literature is not replete with findings relating specific behaviors to obesity in the group targeted for change, so empirical support needs to be generated. Under these circumstances, research should be conducted measuring both the behavior and the selected adiposity indicator(s) in the targeted group in the selected channel. Preliminary qualitative research should be conducted (using focus groups or intensive interviews) of what and how the behaviors are performed in the selected channel (e.g., selection of alternative foods (snack bars) in school lunch in elementary schools), nuances in their performance which may influence mode of measurement (e.g., frequent exchanges of foods between those bringing food from home and those obtaining it at school, which means one can't simply ask a parent what their child ate at school) and cognitive interviews with participants in how they understand the items measuring the behaviors (if some form of self-report is the method selected). The research must clarify whether the primary contributions to adiposity are more likely to occur at school (or at work for adults) or at home[54].

Acceptable levels of quality of measurement and expected relationships should be pre-specified as decision making stop criteria prior to the study. For example, appreciable levels of misclassification error occur at levels of validity below 0.90[55]. While this level is not achievable with current assessment methods, especially with respect to diet and physical activity, there are lower levels of validity at which it would be virtually impossible to detect relationships, or be sensitive to intervention outcomes[56].

Conducting such a study will have the beneficial effects of creating recruitment, measurement and training protocols, demonstrating access to the target population (the ability to recruit a sample), and enabling assessment of possible participation and response biases in collecting such data. Sample descriptive statistics will become known upon which sample size calculations can be made to adequately statistically power an outcome evaluation[57]. Finally, some estimate will be obtained of the level of relationship of the behavior to adiposity, which places an obvious limit on how much change in the adiposity could be obtained in an intervention targeting that behavior. The lack of such a relationship may either suggest it does not exist, or cannot be detected with the methods employed in the targeted group. Either interpretation would question whether the investigators should progress in developing the intervention. Less than acceptable performance on the set criteria of measurement quality will require this Type A study to be improved and repeated, or force the investigators to rethink the target group, target behaviors, and/or data collection procedures.

Funding agencies should be willing to fund such formative research that provides the foundation for an intervention from clearly specified research grant mechanisms.

Formative Step B. Targeted Mediator Validation

With a successful Step A, the investigators must now select the demographic, ecological, social, psychological, and biological variables which will become the mediating and moderating variables for the intervention. One or more behavioral theories should be selected to guide the development of intervention procedures. Particular attention should be paid to documented moderation of mediator-behavior relationships and inconsistent mediation effects (e.g., the presence of competing mechanisms with opposite effects on the outcome)[58]. A balance must be drawn between influences on the individual to change their patterns of behavior, and to change the environment to support (and not subvert) those behavior changes[59], with the possibility of stratifying or exclusively focusing on a specific ethnic group[59], gender, biological influences (e.g., genes[60] or aspects of early growth[61]), or group of individuals with specific environmental, psychosocial, and outcome baseline characteristics[62].

While no one intervention can address all possible influences, the investigative team must select those that theoretically and empirically appear to be the most influential variables for which there are known likely to be effective methods for changing them. In both of our example studies, it is not clear how the intervention specifically capitalized on behavioral theory to promote change in the behaviors[13, 14]. Thus, a conceptual model[63] should be developed that clearly specifies how these influences interrelate and relate to the targeted behavior(s). This should include available estimates of effect sizes for the associations between the mediating variable and the behaviors[64]. Priority should be given to polytheoretical models to maximize predictiveness (especially since this is not meant to be elegant theory testing research), thereby obtaining the biggest handles for changing behavior[65]. The available research should optimally adopt a longitudinal design to explore relationships between changes in mediators and changes in behavior, as cross-sectional and longitudinal associations may differ substantially[66]. A longitudinal research design would also allow testing for temporal stability and for possible exposure effects on interpretation of the survey items[67]. For each of the selected influences, measures must be specified that have been used in the target population with acceptable levels of psychometric characteristics and shown to be predictive of the targeted behavior at levels high enough to expect that change in the mediator will lead to change in the behavior. If such measures have not clearly been validated in comparable target populations, cognitive interviews would be beneficial to assess the target groups' understanding of the items[68].

Most often, quantitative studies have not been conducted of the major predictors of the targeted behavior with the targeted group in the targeted channel. Thereby, new data should be collected starting with preliminary formative qualitative research (e.g., focus groups, intensive interviews).

A sufficiently large sample is necessary to assess the psychometric characteristics of the instruments and to assess the fit of the conceptual model to the data. Preferred methods require that the sample be large enough to conduct the desired analyses in both exploratory and confirmatory subsamples. Investigators should be considering samples of at least 400 to 500 participants, determined in large part by the requirements of the analyses proposed. Optimally, the predictiveness of the model across sub-groups of possible participants should be assessed to verify the need for different approaches to intervention (identification of moderators). Obviously, this would require even larger samples.

The outcomes of this research step will be validation (or need for further validation) of the selected measures of mediating variables; known predictiveness of the selected model with an understanding of which variables are most highly predictive or otherwise centrally involved, and thereby deserving priority in intervention design; and partial information necessary to estimate the sample size to detect mediated effects in the efficacy evaluation.

One of the example studies used measures of knowledge (which have rarely been related to behavior change) with inadequate levels of reliability (rel < 0.55)[13]. Smaller changes in third grade as opposed to 4th or 5th grade knowledge may have been a function of unreliable measurement. The other example study provided no psychometric characteristics on any of the self-reported measures employed[14, 69].

The research team should pre-specify acceptable levels of psychometric characteristics and of levels of predictiveness (e.g., if investigators cannot account for at least 25% of the variance in the behavior, or changes in the mediator yield reliable but trivial changes in the behavior, it is probably not worth proceeding to intervention). Funding agencies should fund Step B projects from funding mechanisms clearly specified for this purpose. This step may need to be repeated if the desired psychometric characteristics or level of predictiveness are not obtained.

Formative Step C. Intervention Procedure Validation

With the clearly specified model, the investigators must identify which mediating variables they will prioritize to change and identify procedures that maximize the likelihood and extent of doing so (i.e. formulate their action theory)[63]. Identifying empirically validated effective mediating variable change procedures may be the hardest issue to find addressed in the published literature. For example, these issues are just now being addressed in the addictive substances literature, and they cannot find patterns in successful versus unsuccessful studies[70]. Most investigators have either used an intuitive approach to specifying how they would change mediators, or post-dicted it, i.e. assessed whether intervention procedures changed the variables they selected for mediators after the intervention was designed (probably on an intuitive basis)[71]. This is not acceptable since it risks capitalizing on chance. The field would benefit from a clearly articulated theory-based taxonomy of change procedures[72] and thorough empirical base evaluating each in diverse demographic groups and channels.

Most investigators will want to focus on several change procedures, at least one for each of the mediating variables, or otherwise the development process will be interminable. The investigators will need a protocol to specify how staff should implement each change procedure, a training manual with certification procedures, and a quality review manual to periodically assess if it is being done as specified for each procedure studied. These manuals should be based on the best understanding of how to get professionals to change their practices, including high specificity[73].

If the literature clearly delineates one or more procedures that would attain acceptable levels of change in the selected mediating variables in the targeted population and channel, and this level meets investigator needs, then the investigator may proceed to Step D. In most cases, however, the investigator will need to design one or more procedures, based on the knowledge of the theory and its mediating variables and of other change procedures (e.g., persuasive messages[74], skill development[75]), and test their effects on the targeted mediating variable(s) in the targeted sample and channel. Elsewhere, this has been called evidentiary research[26]. The investigators should pre-specify a level of change they are willing to accept to progress to the next step for each targeted mediating variable, and below which they are not (because it would compromise the efficacy study). If in step C moderating variables are identified, the investigators need to devise and test separate procedures for each of the subgroups.

The evaluation should include both quantitative measures of the targeted mediating variables and qualitative interviews that assess what participants perceived to be acceptable and unacceptable about the procedure(s), and suggestions for improvement. Funding agencies should fund step C studies from mechanisms clearly specified for this purpose. This step may need to be repeated if the desired level of change is not attained. If no clear refinements of change procedures are available, the investigator may wish to select other mediating variable(s) (with procedures with a likely acceptable level of change) on which to repeat tests of single component intervention procedures. Some investigators with small research capabilities may wish to become purveyors of Step C research, which would still be a substantial contribution to a corpus of intervention research. Funding mechanisms specific to Type C research are to be generated. One of our example studies placed heavy emphasis on increasing knowledge[13], even though a previous thorough review of the literature indicated knowledge change was not related to behavior[7]. The school-based intervention study[13] used procedures that were not demonstrated to influence the corresponding behaviors in the targeted population which has rarely been studied. The evaluation of the media intervention[69] could not differentiate which of the many intervention components contributed to change.

Step D. Pilot/Feasibility Intervention

At this stage, investigators should have a clear idea of what intervention procedures they will use to target each selected mediating variable, along with the associated intervention protocols, staff training procedures, and quality control of implementation procedures. The investigators will also need to address issues in combining the procedures, e.g., sequencing, attaining synergies, and appropriate efficient use of staff and other resources. There has been recent concern that interventions were not delivered as designed (posing problems in evaluating program effects[76]), and that the processes have not been adequately reported [77].

Pilot study evaluation should focus on participation bias (i.e., to whom the intervention appeals), feasibility and process evaluation, ensuring that and assessing whether an adequate dose of intervention with a high enough quality was delivered to an acceptable number of participants (reach)[26]. One of the example studies targeted manual laborers, but more non-manual than manual respondents reported hearing of and seeing the program[14].

A qualitative process evaluation should assess participants' perceptions of what went right, what went wrong, and how it might be improved. Investigators should preset criteria for acceptable process evaluation to enable them to make a decision about whether to proceed to the next step. The pilot study should be long enough for staff to experience the challenges of implementing procedures, and for participants to experience it enough to form opinions, but not necessarily for the full duration in an efficacy study. The process evaluation should be used to further refine the procedures and develop new procedures to address unanticipated problems. One of our example studies recently demonstrated progressively improved compliance with food service guidelines over three years (from 51% to 80% to 87%), but only 56% of schools offering 5 PE sessions per week, and 58% average student attendance at family events[13]. In the other only 17 of 1894 respondents (less than 1%) to a random sample survey sent for a registration pack of intervention materials and only 3 of these actually sent it in[14]. Even the most efficacious interventions will have little effect if inadequately delivered.

Some investigators believe that a single intervention may not be adequate to meet the needs of all possible participants (a "one size fits all" intervention[67]). Some participants may need extra dose, or different types of dose, at certain points in the intervention, while highly motivated others may need little more than encouragement or guidance. This attempt at developing an intervention to meet these diverse needs in systematic ways has been termed a branched logic or stepped intervention[78]. The result of step D could be sufficient qualitative with some quantitative data to design such a branched logic intervention. If new procedures are needed, the investigators may need to repeat a Step C study with the subgroup. This option would require an ensuing pilot study to pilot test the implementation of the branched processes before moving on to a Multiphase Optimization STrategy (MOST) program evaluation[78].

Brief Comments on Intervention Efficacy and Effectiveness Trials

At this point the investigators will have protocols for and substantial experience in all implementation, intervention, and measurement procedures. An efficacy trial is the next step. The efficacy trial tests if a theory-based intervention will work under ideal circumstances (i.e., when adequate resources are available to deliver the intervention exactly as designed with adequate time to deliver it). Efficacy trials have been discussed at length[11, 79, 80], but a few issues should be pointed out.

A randomized clinical trial is the obvious preferred research design for an efficacy trial. Since science will benefit from understanding both what procedures worked and the corresponding processes of change[81], an appropriate evaluation will include both a process evaluation and a mediating variable analysis[82]. Targeted mediated and outcome variables should be measured as frequently as possible to assess change, their time course, and to relate change in mediators to both delivered dose and change in outcomes[82]. A qualitative process evaluation should assess participants' perceptions of the acceptability of each intervention component, and how it might be improved. Evaluation of a MOST trial would require even more complex evaluation procedures[78, 83]. Cost effectiveness studies are inappropriate at this step because the trial was not designed with cost effectiveness considerations in mind. If the intervention does not work in efficacy circumstances, it is very unlikely to work under other less carefully formulated circumstances and so no further studies should be contemplated.

There is an emerging issue that self-report measures could be influenced by repeated use in the same group, or by participating in an intervention[26, 84]. This would be the first level of study where these issues could be clearly addressed. Efficacy trials should be reported using CONSORT[85], or other trial reporting protocols[86, 87], to assure their optimal contribution to the literature.

Some large scale "efficacy" type intervention trials have changed intervention procedures in the midst of the intervention trial, as some procedures did not seem to work, and/or others appeared to offer more promise[26]. While this may make some practical sense to not lose the benefit of an expensive intervention, it makes interpreting the experiment challenging. Conducting studies A through D with possible repeats to work out the "bugs", should minimize the urge to change intervention procedures in midstream, and thereby provide clearer tests of intervention outcomes.

Most programs in the non-research world do not have the resources (e.g., adequate number of staff with all necessary expertise, adequate resources, sufficient time) to deliver interventions under efficacy circumstances. An effectiveness trial tests whether the principles learned from successful efficacy trials can be implemented to good effect under more "real world" circumstances. A number of investigators have addressed the design of effectiveness type studies [88–90]. Cost effectiveness analyses make most sense when conducted with effectiveness trials, because issues of cost will determine if the intervention (or components thereof) will be disseminated. The CONSORT or similar reporting protocols should be used for reporting effectiveness studies, as well.

An assumption of the proposed approach is that the mediating variable model is valid in accounting for how interventions work. The key assumption is that changes in mediators account for relatively stable changes in behaviors in an approximately linear fashion. Alternative models would be the incentive model[91], i.e. change occurs only in response to tangible incentives; a tipping point model[92], where there is some minimally necessary level of the mediating variable, before which no behavior change occurs; or an activation model, i.e. people with high level of a mediating variable that is "dormant" or "unattended to" have that variable "activated"[93], while only those with low levels of the mediator require its change. (These are all examples of nonlinear or unstable mediating variable relationships.). There do not appear to be enough behavior therapists in the world to manage the contingent delivery of incentives to broadly deliver the incentive model. Little is known about how to activate mediating variables, or measure this dual activation and mediation process. But, if necessary, this could be an area for future research. An issue deserving more attention in mounting such efforts is the organizational structure, number and expertise of staff, linkages to the target group(s) and channels, and many other leadership, infrastructure, and staff motivation issues[94].

The main drawbacks associated with this approach to program development include the extended time and resources needed to complete the various steps in the process. While true, not following a logical sequence of actions that identifies the best available behavioral, social, and ecological theories and methods to tackle obesity in particular populations and settings, we maximize the risk of wasting time and resources. The current state of affairs is testimony to these concerns.

Conclusion

Intervention researchers and practitioners need to engage in a process to design and develop interventions that maximize their programs' likely effects and what can be learned from such trials. Funding agencies also need to have a clear idea of a process for program development to know how to support these important activities. This manuscript proposes such a process. Further discussion should be stimulated among the various interested parties, and hopefully a consensus will emerge on the optimal processes in which to engage and support.

Abbreviations

BMI:

Body Mass Index

MOST:

Multiple phase Optimization STrategy, a type of program evaluation

CONSORT:

CONsolidated Standards Of Reporting Trials.

References

  1. Ogden C, Carroll M, Curtin L, McDowell M, Tabak C, Flegal K: Prevalence of overweight in the United States, 1999–2004. JAMA. 2006, 1549-1555. 10.1001/jama.295.13.1549.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Popkin BM: Will China's nutrition transition overwhelm its health care system and slow economic growth?. Health Aff (Millwood). 2008, 27 (4): 1064-1076. 10.1377/hlthaff.27.4.1064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Mozaffarian D, Wilson PW, Kannel WB: Beyond established and novel risk factors: lifestyle risk factors for cardiovascular disease. Circulation. 2008, 117 (23): 3031-3038. 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.738732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Summerbell C, Waters E, Edmunds L, Kelly S, Brown T, Campbell K: Interventions for preventing obesity in children. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2005, CD001871-3

  5. Ammerman AS, Lindquist CH, Lohr KN, Hersey J: The efficacy of behavioral interventions to modify dietary fat and fruit and vegetable intake: A review of the evidence. Prev Med. 2002, 35 (1): 25-41. 10.1006/pmed.2002.1028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Lemmens VE, Oenema A, Klepp KI, Henriksen HB, Brug J: A systematic review of the evidence regarding efficacy of obesity prevention interventions among adults. Obes Rev. 2008

    Google Scholar 

  7. Contento I, Balch GI, Bronner YL, Lytle L, Maloney SK, Olson CM, Swadener SS: The effectiveness of nutrition education and implications for nutrition education policy, programs, and research: A review of research. J Nutr Educ. 1995, 27: 277-418.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Laven GT: Insufficient evidence for committee recommendations on obesity. Pediatrics. 2008, 121 (5): 1077-1078. 10.1542/peds.2008-0003. author reply 1078–1079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kumanyika SK, Obarzanek E, Stettler N, Bell R, Field AE, Fortmann SP, Franklin BA, Gillman MW, Lewis CE, Poston WC, Stevens J, Hong Y: Population-based prevention of obesity. The need for comprehensive promotion of healthful eating, physical activity, and energy balance. A scientific statement from American Heart Association Council on Epidemiology and Prevention, Interdisciplinary Committee for Prevention (Formerly the Expert Panel on Population and Prevention Science). Circulation. 2008

    Google Scholar 

  10. Kok G: Quality of planning as a decisive determinant of health education effectiveness. Hygie. 1992, 11 (4): 5-9.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Hardeman W, Sutton S, Griffin S, Johnston M, White A, Wareham NJ, Kinmonth AL: A causal modelling approach to the development of theory-based behaviour change programmes for trial evaluation. Health Educ Res. 2005, 20 (6): 676-687. 10.1093/her/cyh022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Campbell NC, Murray E, Darbyshire J, Emery J, Farmer A, Griffiths F, Guthrie B, Lester H, Wilson P, Kinmonth AL: Designing and evaluating complex interventions to improve health care. BMJ. 2007, 334 (7591): 455-459. 10.1136/bmj.39108.379965.BE.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Caballero B, Clay T, Davis SM, Ethelbah B, Rock BH, Lohman T, Norman J, Story M, Stone EJ, Stephenson L, Stevens J: Pathways: a school-based, randomized controlled trial for the prevention of obesity in American Indian schoolchildren. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003, 78 (5): 1030-1038.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Wardle J, Rapoport L, Miles A, Afuape T, Duman M: Mass education for obesity prevention: the penetration of the BBC's 'Fighting Fat, Fighting Fit' campaign. Health Educ Res. 2001, 16 (3): 343-355. 10.1093/her/16.3.343.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Baranowski T, Lin LS, Wetter DW, Resnicow K, Hearn MD: Theory as mediating variables: why aren't community interventions working as desired?. Ann Epidemiol. 1997, 7: S89-S95. 10.1016/S1047-2797(97)80011-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Bachman CM, Baranowski T, Nicklas TA: Is there an association between sweetened beverages and adiposity?. Nutr Rev. 2006, 64 (4): 153-174. 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2006.tb00199.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Bar-Or O, Baranowski T: Physical activity, obesity and adiposity among adolescents. Pediatric Exercise Science. 1994, 348-360. 6

  18. Foster E, Matthews JN, Lloyd J, Marshall L, Mathers JC, Nelson M, Barton KL, Wrieden WL, Cornelissen P, Harris J, Adamson AJ: Children's estimates of food portion size: the development and evaluation of three portion size assessment tools for use with children. Br J Nutr. 2008, 99 (1): 175-184.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Haerens L, Cerin E, Deforche B, Maes L, De Bourdeaudhuij I: Explaining the effects of a 1-year intervention promoting a low fat diet in adolescent girls: a mediation analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2007, 4: 55-10.1186/1479-5868-4-55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Watson K, Baranowski T, Thompson D: Item response modeling: an evaluation of the children's fruit and vegetable self-efficacy questionnaire. Health Educ Res. 2006, 21 (Suppl 1): i47-57. 10.1093/her/cyl136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Baranowski T, Anderson C, Carmack C: Mediating variable framework in physical activity interventions. How are we doing? How might we do better?. Am J Prev Med. 1998, 15 (4): 266-297. 10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00080-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Resnicow K, Davis M, Smith M, Lazarus-Yaroch A, Baranowski T, Baranowski J, Doyle C, Wang DT: How best to measure implementation of health curricula: A comparison of three measures. Health Educ Res. 1998, 13: 239-250. 10.1093/her/13.2.239.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Glanz K: Dietary Change. Cancer Cause Control. 1997, 8 (Suppl 1): S13-16.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Gucciardini E, Cameron J, Liao C, Palmer A, Steward D: Program design features that can imporve participation in health education interventions. BMC Health Res Methodol. 2007, 7: 47-10.1186/1471-2288-7-47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Garnett SP, Baur LA, Cowell CT: Waist-to-height ratio: a simple option for determining excess central adiposity in young people. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008, 32 (6): 1028-1030. 10.1038/ijo.2008.51.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Stevens J, Taber DR, Murray DM, Ward DS: Advances and controversies in the design of obesity prevention trials. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2007, 15 (9): 2163-2170. 10.1038/oby.2007.257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Prentice AM, Jebb SA: Beyond body mass index. Obes Rev. 2001, 2 (3): 141-147. 10.1046/j.1467-789x.2001.00031.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Romero-Corral A, Somers VK, Sierra-Johnson J, Thomas RJ, Collazo-Clavell ML, Korinek J, Allison TG, Batsis JA, Sert-Kuniyoshi FH, Lopez-Jimenez F: Accuracy of body mass index in diagnosing obesity in the adult general population. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008, 32 (6): 959-966. 10.1038/ijo.2008.11.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Burkhauser RV, Cawley J, Beyond BMI: the value of more accurate measures of fatness and obesity in social science research. J Health Econ. 2008, 27 (2): 519-529. 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2007.05.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Cole TJ, Faith MS, Pietrobelli A, Heo M: What is the best measure of adiposity change in growing children: BMI, BMI %, BMI z-score or BMI centile?. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2005, 59 (3): 419-425. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602090.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Lohman TG, Caballero B, Himes JH, Davis CE, Stewart D, Houtkooper L, Going SB, Hunsberger S, Weber JL, Reid R, Stephenson L: Estimation of body fat from anthropometry and bioelectrical impedance in Native American children. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2000, 24 (8): 982-988. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801318.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Schatzkin A, Kipnis V: Could exposure assessment problems give us wrong answers to nutrition and cancer questions?. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2004, 96 (21): 1564-1565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Golay A, Bobbioni E: The role of dietary fat in obesity. Int J Obes. 1997, 21 (Suppl 3): S2-S11.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Bessesen DH, Bull S, Cornier MA: Trafficking of dietary fat and resistance to obesity. Physiol Behav. 2008, 94 (5): 681-688. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.04.019.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Jago R, Baranowski T, Baranowski JC, Thompson D, Greaves KA: BMI from 3–6 y of age is predicted by TV viewing and physical activity, not diet. Int J Obes (Lond). 2005, 29 (6): 557-564. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802969.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Ruottinen S, Niinikoski H, Lagstrom H, Ronnemaa T, Hakanen M, Viikari J, Jokinen E, Simell O: High sucrose intake is associated with poor quality of diet and growth between 13 months and 9 years of age: the special Turku Coronary Risk Factor Intervention Project. Pediatrics. 2008, 121 (6): e1676-1685. 10.1542/peds.2007-1642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Newby PK, Muller D, Hallfrisch J, Andres R, Tucker KL: Food patterns measured by factor analysis and anthropometric changes in adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004, 80 (2): 504-513.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Newby PK, Weismayer C, Akesson A, Tucker KL, Wolk A: Longitudinal changes in food patterns predict changes in weight and body mass index and the effects are greatest in obese women. J Nutr. 2006, 136 (10): 2580-2587.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Murtaugh MA, Sweeney C, Giuliano AR, Herrick JS, Hines L, Byers T, Baumgartner KB, Slattery ML: Diet patterns and breast cancer risk in Hispanic and non-Hispanic white women: the Four-Corners Breast Cancer Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008, 87 (4): 978-984.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Shi Z, Hu X, Yuan B, Hu G, Pan X, Dai Y, Byles JE, Holmboe-Ottesen G: Vegetable-rich food pattern is related to obesity in China. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008, 32 (6): 975-984. 10.1038/ijo.2008.21.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Ayala GX, Rogers M, Arredondo EM, Campbell NR, Baquero B, Duerksen SC, Elder JP: Away-from-home food intake and risk for obesity: examining the influence of context. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2008, 16 (5): 1002-1008. 10.1038/oby.2008.34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Strong WB, Malina RM, Blimkie CJ, Daniels SR, Dishman RK, Gutin B, Hergenroeder AC, Must A, Nixon PA, Pivarnik JM, Rowland T, Trost S, Trudeau F: Evidence based physical activity for school-age youth. J Pediatr. 2005, 146 (6): 732-737. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.01.055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Metcalf BS, Voss LD, Hosking J, Jeffery AN, Wilkin TJ: Physical activity at the government-recommended level and obesity-related health outcomes: a longitudinal study (EarlyBird 37). Arch Dis Child. 2008

    Google Scholar 

  44. Ekelund U: Cardiorespiratory fitness, exercise capacity and physical activity in children: are we measuring the right thing?. Arch Dis Child. 2008, 93 (6): 455-456. 10.1136/adc.2007.135202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Borradaile KE, Foster GD, May H, Karpyn A, Sherman S, Grundy K, Nachmani J, Vander Veur S, Boruch RF: Associations between the Youth/Adolescent Questionnaire, the Youth/Adolescent Activity Questionnaire, and body mass index z score in low-income inner-city fourth through sixth grade children. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008, 87 (6): 1650-1655.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Nicklas TA, Yang SJ, Baranowski T, Zakeri I, Berenson G: Eating patterns and obesity in children. The Bogalusa Heart Study. Am J Prev Med. 2003, 25 (1): 9-16. 10.1016/S0749-3797(03)00098-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Keith SW, Redden DT, Katzmarzyk PT, Boggiano MM, Hanlon EC, Benca RM, Ruden D, Pietrobelli A, Barger JL, Fontaine KR, Wang C, Aronne LJ, Wright SM, Baskin M, Dhurandhar NV, et al: Putative contributors to the secular increase in obesity: exploring the roads less traveled. Int J Obes (Lond). 2006, 30 (11): 1585-1594. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803326.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Carnell S, Wardle J: Measuring behavioural susceptibility to obesity: validation of the child eating behaviour questionnaire. Appetite. 2007, 48 (1): 104-113. 10.1016/j.appet.2006.07.075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Ebbeling CB, Sinclair KB, Pereira MA, Garcia-Lago E, Feldman HA, Ludwig DS: Compensation for energy intake from fast food among overweight and lean adolescents. J Am Med Assoc. 2004, 291 (23): 2828-2833. 10.1001/jama.291.23.2828.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Melzer K, Kayser B, Saris WH, Pichard C: Effects of physical activity on food intake. Clin Nutr. 2005, 24 (6): 885-895. 10.1016/j.clnu.2005.06.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Rockett H: Commentary on lack of association between television viewing, soft drinks, physical activity and body mass index in children. Acta Paediatr. 2008, 97 (6): 699-700. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00817.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Baranowski T: Advances in basic behavioral research will make the most important contributions to effective dietary change programs at this time. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006, 106 (6): 808-811. 10.1016/j.jada.2006.03.032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Bouchard C: The magnitude of the energy imbalance in obesity is generally underestimated. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008, 32 (6): 879-880. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803796.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Gutin B, Yin Z, Johnson M, Barbeau P: Preliminary findings of the effect of a 3-year after-school physical activity intervention on fitness and body fat: the Medical College of Georgia Fitkid Project. Int J Pediatr Obes. 2008, 3 (Suppl 1): 3-9. 10.1080/17477160801896457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. de Moor C, Baranowski T, Cullen KW, Nicklas T: Misclassification associated with measurement error in the assessment of dietary intake. Pub Health Nutr. 2003, 6: 393-399. 10.1079/PHN2002446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Cerin E, Baranowski T: Impact of unreliability of behavior and adiposity measures on outcomes of obesity prevention and treatment programs. Intern J Obes.

  57. Murray D, Catellier D, Hannan P, Treuth M, Steven J, Schmitz K, Rice J, Conway T: School-level intraclass correlations for physical activity in adolescent girls. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2004, 36: 876-882. 10.1249/01.MSS.0000126806.72453.1C.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Kumanyika SK: Environmental influences on childhood obesity: ethnic and cultural influences in context. Physiol Behav. 2008, 94 (1): 61-70. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.019.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Share M, Strain M: Making schools and young people responsible: a critical analysis of Ireland's obesity strategy. Health Soc Care Community. 2008, 16 (3): 234-243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Fernandez JR, Casazza K, Divers J, Lopez-Alarcon M: Disruptions in energy balance: does nature overcome nurture?. Physiol Behav. 2008, 94 (1): 105-112. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.021.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Adair LS: Child and adolescent obesity: epidemiology and developmental perspectives. Physiol Behav. 2008, 94 (1): 8-16. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.016.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  62. King AC, Marcus B, Ahn D, Dunn AL, Rejeski WJ, Sallis JF, Coday M: Identifying subgroups that succeed or fail with three levels of physical activity intervention: the Activity Counseling Trial. Health Psychol. 2006, 25 (3): 336-347. 10.1037/0278-6133.25.3.336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Chen H: Theory-Driven Evaluations. 1990, Newbury Park, CA: Sage

    Google Scholar 

  64. MacKinnon D: Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis. 2008, New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  65. Michie S, Johnston M, Abraham C, Lawton R, Parker D, Walker A: Making psychological theory useful for implementing evidence based practice: a consensus approach. Qual Saf Health Care. 2005, 14 (1): 26-33. 10.1136/qshc.2004.011155.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Szklo M, Nieto F: Epidemiology: Beyond the Basics. 2000, Gaithersbury, MD: Aspen Publications

    Google Scholar 

  67. Baranowski T, Allen DD, Masse LC, Wilson M: Does participation in an intervention affect responses on self-report questionnaires?. Health Educ Res. 2006, 21 (Suppl 1): i98-109. 10.1093/her/cyl087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Carbone ET, Campbell MK, Honess-Morreale L: Use of cognitive interview techniques in the development of nutrition surveys and interactive nutrition messages for low-income populations. J Am Diet Assoc. 2002, 102 (5): 690-696. 10.1016/S0002-8223(02)90156-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Miles A, Rapoport L, Wardle J, Afuape T, Duman M: Using the mass-media to target obesity: an analysis of the characteristics and reported behaviour change of participants in the BBC's 'Fighting Fat, Fighting Fit' campaign. Health Educ Res. 2001, 16 (3): 357-372. 10.1093/her/16.3.357.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  70. Hyde J, Hankins M, Deale A, Marteau TM: Interventions to increase self-efficacy in the context of addiction behaviours: A systematic literature review. J Health Psychol. 2008, 13 (5): 607-623. 10.1177/1359105308090933.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  71. Michie S, Abraham C: Interventions to change health behaviors: Evidence-based or evidence-inspired?. Psychol Health. 2004, 19 (1): 29-49. 10.1080/0887044031000141199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Michie S: Designing and implementing behavior change interventions to imporve population health. J Health Serv Res Policy. 2008, 1-6.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Michie S, Johnston M: Changing clinical behaviour by making guidelines specific. Bmj. 2004, 328 (7435): 343-345. 10.1136/bmj.328.7435.343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. van Assema P, Martens M, Ruiter RA, Brug J: Framing of nutrition education messages in persuading consumers of the advantages of a healthy diet. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2001, 14 (6): 435-442. 10.1046/j.1365-277X.2001.00315.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  75. Cullen K, Baranowski T, Smith S: Using goal setting as a strategy for dietary behavior change. J Am Diet Assoc. 2001, 101: 562-566. 10.1016/S0002-8223(01)00140-7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Michie S, Hardeman W, Fanshawe T, Prevost AT, Taylor L, Kinmonth AL: Investigating theoretical explanations for behavior change: The case study ProActive. Psychol Health. 2008, 23 (1): 25-39. 10.1080/08870440701670588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Riley BL, Macdonald J, Mansi O, Kothari A, Kurtz D, Vontettenborn LI, Edwards NC: Is reporting on interventions a weak link in understanding how and why they work? A preliminary exploration using community heart health exemplars. Implement Sci. 2008, 3: 27-10.1186/1748-5908-3-27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Collins LM, Murphy SA, Nair VN, Strecher VJ: A strategy for optimizing and evaluating behavioral interventions. Ann Behav Med. 2005, 30 (1): 65-73. 10.1207/s15324796abm3001_8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Campbell MK, et al: The North Carolina Black Churches United for Better Health Project: Intervention and process evaluation. Health Educ Behav. 2000, (this issue).

    Google Scholar 

  80. Kropski JA, Keckley PH, Jensen GL: School-based obesity prevention programs: an evidence-based review. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2008, 16 (5): 1009-1018. 10.1038/oby.2008.29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Michie S: What works and how? Designing more effective interventions needs answers to both questions. Addiction. 2008, 103 (6): 886-887. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.02112.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Baranowski T, Jago R: Understanding mechanisms of change in children's physical activity programs. Exercise Sport Sci R. 2005, 33 (4): 163-168. 10.1097/00003677-200510000-00003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. West SG, Aiken LS: Toward understanding individual effects in multicomponent prevention programs: Design and analysis strategies. In: Bryant KJ, Windle M, West SG, eds. The Science of Prevention, Methodological Advances from Alcohol and Substance Abuse Research. 1997, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association

    Google Scholar 

  84. Baranowski T, Allen D, Masse L, Wilson M: Does participation in an intervention affect responses on self report questionnaires?. Health Educ Res. 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Moher D, Schulz KF, Altman DG: The CONSORT statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of parallel-group randomized trials. Lancet. 2001, 357 (9263): 1191-1194. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)04337-3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  86. Dixon JR: The International Conference on Harmonization Good Clinical Practice guideline. Qual Assur. 1998, 6 (2): 65-74.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Des Jarlais DC, Lyles C, Crepaz N: Improving the reporting quality of nonrandomized evaluations of behavioral and public health interventions: the TREND statement. Am J Public Health. 2004, 94 (3): 361-366. 10.2105/AJPH.94.3.361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Glasgow RE, Magid DJ, Beck A, Ritzwoller D, Estabrooks PA: Practical clinical trials for translating research to practice: design and measurement recommendations. Med Care. 2005, 43 (6): 551-557. 10.1097/01.mlr.0000163645.41407.09.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Klesges LM, Estabrooks PA, Dzewaltowski DA, Bull SS, Glasgow RE: Beginning with the application in mind: Designing and planning health behavior change interventions to enhance dissemination. Ann Behav Med. 2005, 29: 66-75. 10.1207/s15324796abm2902s_10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Dzewaltowski DA, Noble JM, Shaw JM: Physical activity participation: Social cognitive theory versus the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior. J Sport Exercise Psychol. 1990, 12: 288-405.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Godley SH, Godley MD, Wright KL, Funk RR, Petry NM: Contingent reinforcement of personal goal activities for adolescents with substance use disorders during post-residential continuing care. Am J Addict. 2008, 17 (4): 278-286. 10.1080/10550490802138798.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Boyatzis RE: Using tipping points of emotional intelligence and cognitive competencies to predict financial performance of leaders. Psicothema. 2006, 18 (Suppl): 124-131.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Herry C, Ciocchi S, Senn V, Demmou L, Muller C, Luthi A: Switching on and off fear by distinct neuronal circuits. Nature. 2008

    Google Scholar 

  94. Anderson D, Raine KD, Plotnikoff RC, Cook K, Barrett L, Smith C: aBaseline assessment of organizational capacity for health promotion within regional health authorities in Alberta, Canada. Promot Educ. 2008, 15 (2): 6-14. 10.1177/1025382308090339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was primarily funded by a grant from the National Institute of Diabetes & Digestive & Kidney Diseases (5 U44 DK66724-01). This work is also a publication of the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA/ARS) Children's Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, and had been funded in part with federal funds from the USDA/ARS under Cooperative Agreement No. 58-6250-6001. The contents of this publication do not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the USDA, nor does mention of trade names, commercial products, or organizations imply endorsement from the U.S. government.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tom Baranowski.

Additional information

Competing interests

Tom Baranowski serves on the Global Advisory Committee for the McDonald's Corporation. The authors have no other competing interests.

Authors' contributions

TB wrote a first draft of this manuscript. EC made numerous contributions to ensuing drafts. JB made intellectual contributions throughout the development of this manuscript, and edited a near final draft.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baranowski, T., Cerin, E. & Baranowski, J. Steps in the design, development and formative evaluation of obesity prevention-related behavior change trials. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 6, 6 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-6-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-6-6

Keywords