Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Feb;34(1):17-24.
doi: 10.1037/pag0000288. Epub 2018 Sep 13.

Trajectories of normal cognitive aging

Affiliations

Trajectories of normal cognitive aging

Timothy A Salthouse. Psychol Aging. 2019 Feb.

Abstract

Although sensitive detection of pathological cognitive aging requires accurate information about the trajectory of normal cognitive aging, prior research has revealed inconsistent patterns of age-cognition relations with cross-sectional and longitudinal comparisons. Age trends in four cognitive domains were compared in over 5,000 adults with cross-sectional data, and in almost 1,600 adults with three-occasion longitudinal data. Quasi-longitudinal comparisons, which are similar to cross-sectional comparisons in that there is no prior test experience and are similar to longitudinal comparisons in that the participants are from the same birth cohorts, were also reported. The age trends in quasi-longitudinal comparisons more closely resembled those in cross-sectional comparisons than those in longitudinal comparisons, which suggests that, at least up until about age 65, age-cognition relations in longitudinal comparisons are distorted by prior test experience. Results from cross-sectional and quasi-longitudinal comparisons, which can be assumed to have minimal test experience effects, imply that normal cognitive aging is characterized by nearly linear declines from early adulthood in speed, and accelerating declines in memory and reasoning. However, vocabulary knowledge increased until the decade of the 60's in all three types of comparisons. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1 –
Figure 1 –
Means and standard errors of the cross-sectional and three-occasion longitudinal data, and estimates of quasi-longitudinal relations in four cognitive domains. The quasi-longitudinal trajectories are portrayed as originating at the first longitudinal occasion, and extending over an interval equal to the average longitudinal interval. Quasi-longitudinal values are only reported for the Time 1 and Time 3 occasions to minimize clutter in the figures.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alder AG, Adam J, & Arenberg D (1990). Individual-differences assessment of the relationship between change in and initial level of adult cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging, 5, 560–568. - PubMed
    1. Bielak AAM Anstey KJ, Christensen H, & Windsor TD (2012). Activity engagement is related to level, but not change in cognitive ability across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 27, 219–228. - PubMed
    1. Broadbent DE & Gregory M (1965). Some confirmatory results on age differences in memory for simultaneous stimulation. British Journal of Psychology, 56, 77–80. - PubMed
    1. Caselli RJ, Dueck AC, Osborn D, Sabbagh MN, Connor DJ, Ahern GL, Baxter LC, Rapcsak SZ, Shi J, Woodruff BK, Locke DEC, Snyder CH, Alexander GE, Rademakers R, & Reiman EM (2009). Longitudinal modeling of age-related memory decline and the APOE e4 effect. New England Journal of Medicine, 361, 255–263. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, & Dermen D (1976). Manual for Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.